Anteprima
Vedrai una selezione di 10 pagine su 41
Lezioni di differenze individuali nel temperaemento e personalità (parte prima) Pag. 1 Lezioni di differenze individuali nel temperaemento e personalità (parte prima) Pag. 2
Anteprima di 10 pagg. su 41.
Scarica il documento per vederlo tutto.
Lezioni di differenze individuali nel temperaemento e personalità (parte prima) Pag. 6
Anteprima di 10 pagg. su 41.
Scarica il documento per vederlo tutto.
Lezioni di differenze individuali nel temperaemento e personalità (parte prima) Pag. 11
Anteprima di 10 pagg. su 41.
Scarica il documento per vederlo tutto.
Lezioni di differenze individuali nel temperaemento e personalità (parte prima) Pag. 16
Anteprima di 10 pagg. su 41.
Scarica il documento per vederlo tutto.
Lezioni di differenze individuali nel temperaemento e personalità (parte prima) Pag. 21
Anteprima di 10 pagg. su 41.
Scarica il documento per vederlo tutto.
Lezioni di differenze individuali nel temperaemento e personalità (parte prima) Pag. 26
Anteprima di 10 pagg. su 41.
Scarica il documento per vederlo tutto.
Lezioni di differenze individuali nel temperaemento e personalità (parte prima) Pag. 31
Anteprima di 10 pagg. su 41.
Scarica il documento per vederlo tutto.
Lezioni di differenze individuali nel temperaemento e personalità (parte prima) Pag. 36
Anteprima di 10 pagg. su 41.
Scarica il documento per vederlo tutto.
Lezioni di differenze individuali nel temperaemento e personalità (parte prima) Pag. 41
1 su 41
D/illustrazione/soddisfatti o rimborsati
Disdici quando
vuoi
Acquista con carta
o PayPal
Scarica i documenti
tutte le volte che vuoi
Estratto del documento

A Definition of Temperament

Gordon Allport (1961) defined temperament as "the characteristic phenomena of an individual's emotional nature, including his susceptibility to emotional stimulation, his customary strength and speed of response, the quality of his prevailing mood, these phenomena being regarded as dependent upon constitutional make-up and, therefore, largely hereditary in origin".

Allport's definition focused on individual differences in emotional reactivity. Thomas and Chess (1977), however, took a broader approach to temperament, including individual differences in attention and activity level. Taking into account both of these approaches, we have defined temperament as constitutionally based individual differences in reactivity and self-regulation, in the domains of affect, activity, and attention.

The term "constitutional" refers to the biological bases of temperament, influenced over time by heredity, maturation, and

Experience and self-regulation are umbrella terms that broadly organize the temperament domain. Reactivity refers to responsiveness to change in the external and internal environment. Reactivity includes a broad range of reactions (e.g., the emotions of fear, cardiac reactivity) and more general tendencies (e.g., negative emotionality), thus it is not limited to general reactivity.

Temperament describes tendencies or dispositions that are not continually expressed but require appropriate eliciting conditions.

Temperament and Personality

Temperament represents the affective, activational, and attentional core of personality, whereas personality includes much more than temperament, particularly the content of thought, skills, habits, values, morals, beliefs, and social cognition. Social cognition includes the perception of the self, others, and the relation of self to objects, events, and others. Over time, social cognition becomes increasingly important in eliciting and moderating temperamental.

Personality traits have been defined as patterns of thoughts, emotion, and behavior that show consistency across situations and stability over time, and that "affect the individual's getting along with other people and with himself".

Temperament traits similarly show consistency across situations and stability over time, but they are limited to basic processes of reactivity and self-regulation, and do not include the specific content of thought or the use of conceptually based defences (e.g., paranoia).

HISTORY OF TEMPERAMENT: RESEARCH ON CHILDHOOD

Several lines of inquiry have contributed to contemporary temperament research on children. One is the research of the normative child psychologists in the 1920s and 1930s, who observed large numbers of children to establish the normal sequences of motor and mental development and studied small samples of children intensively over time. In doing so, they noted striking temperamental variability among the children they observed.

Shirley's intensive study of the motor development of a group of infants during the first 2 years led to her observation of the infant's "core of personality." Shirley (1933) noted that developmentally, "Both constancy and change characterize the personality of the baby. Traits are constant enough to make it plausible that a nucleus of personality exists at birth and that this nucleus persists and grows and determines to a certain degree the relative importance of (other) traits. Three important concepts: 1) First, temperament traits are constitutionally based characteristics that provide the core of personality and influence directions for development. 2) Second, although some stability of temperament is expected across age, developmental outcomes will also depend on the child's experience in the social context. Finally, as in the case of C. D., given set of temperamental characteristics 3) allows for multiple possible outcomes. Different trajectories and

Outcomes may occur for children with similar temperamental traits, and children differing in temperament may come to similar developmental outcomes via different pathways (Kochanska, 1995).

A second major line of research on temperament in childhood came from biologically oriented clinicians. Bergman and Escalona (1949) identified children who were particularly reactive to low intensities of stimulation in one or more sensory modalities. In Escalona's (1968) ground-breaking book, The Roots of Individuality, she proposed "effective experience," the concept of the idea that events in children's lives are experienced only as they are filtered through the individual child's nervous system. A given event will thus differ in its effects for children who differ in temperament.

Inspired by differences among their own children, Chess and Thomas studied "primary reaction patterns," individual differences in what they called the collecting interviews.

from parents of infants on repeated occasions. Beginning when their initial sample of 22 infants was 3 to 6 months of age, parents were extensively interviewed about their infants' behavior in varying contexts. Each infant reaction and its context was then typed on a separate sheet of paper, and Birch inductively sorted the descriptions into categories that came to represent the nine NYLS temperament dimensions. (1) Activity Level, (2) Approach/ Withdrawal, (3) Adaptability, (4) Mood, (5) Threshold, (6) Intensity, (7) Distractibility, (8) Rhythmicity, and (9) Attention Span / Persistence. Later, Michael Rutter suggested the term temperament to describe their area of study, and this term was adopted by the NYLS group.

Acceptance of Temperament Research Finally, cognitive approaches stressed children's influences on their own development via their perceptual and cognitive mental representations of events (Kohlberg, 1969). Research on temperament would now introduce the idea that,

In addition to individual differences in thought patterns, individual differences in children's emotional processing could bias their affective representations of experience, with important implications for their development. Eastern and Western Schools of Temperament and Personality Dimensions emerged from early factor-analytic studies of temperament in adults. In Great Britain, Webb (1915) analysed self-report items referring to emotionality, activity, qualities of the self, and intelligence and thus identified two broad factors. 1) One, labeled "w," was defined as "consistency of action resulting from deliberate volition or will". This factor bears similarities to temperamental Effortful Control in childhood, and to the higher-order personality factors more recently labelled Control, Constraint, or Conscientiousness. 2) A second factor assessed distress proneness or negative emotionality, sometimes labelled emotional stability-instability; Eysenck (1967) wouldlatercall it By 1937, Burt had also identified the factor of , and later factor-analytic research on questionnaire assessed personality has repeatedly , , and . identified three factors: These three factors, sometimes called the , along with factors of and , have been extracted from factor analyses of trait descriptive words and personality items, and constitute what have been called the personality factors. THE STRUCTURE OF TEMPERAMENT Consider research on temperament that has led to revision of the original list of nine NYLS dimensions. Much of this research employed factor analysis of large sets of items within the temperament domain. Factor analysis allows researchers to see simultaneously the relations and nonrelations among large sets of behavior descriptions. A major limitation of the factor analytic method is that the dimensions yielded by the analysis depend on

The descriptors included in the initial datamatrix. Several broad dimensions of temperament have consistently emerged from different sets of data.

Infant Studies

In a review of the structure of temperament as indicated by infant studies, six dimensions were identified that provided a shorter list of temperament variables for future researchers (Table).

  1. Individual differences in positive emotionality were differentiated from negative emotionality, and two kinds of negative emotion were identified: fear and anger/irritable distress.
  2. The second shared dimension was Irritable Distress, assessed by IBQ Distress to Limitations, RITQ Negative Mood (which includes positive affect at one pole), and ICQ Fussy/Difficult scales.
  3. The third shared temperament dimension was Activity Level, assessed only on the RITQ and IBQ scales, where the correlation for both mothers and day-care teachers was .65.

Conclusions: First, the structure appears to correspond more to dimensions of reactivity in the basic

emotions and attention /regulation than to a general style. Second, bipolar constructs such as approach versus withdrawal and good versus bad mood have not emerged from these analyses; instead, unipolar constructs of infant temperament have gained support. Third, these dimensions also correspond to individual differences emerging from studies of nonhuman animals(Gosling & John, 1999), allowing links between temperament constructs in humans and the psychobiology of individual differences.

Childhood Studies

Factor analyses of questionnaire items based on the NYLS for older children have similarly revealed as shorter list of broad temperament factors (Table 3.2). Analysis of mother reports for 3- to 8-year-olds on the Thomas and Chess (1977) Childhood Temperament Questionnaire in the Australian Temperament Project (ATP) produced factors of Inflexibility (irritability and uncooperativeness), Persistence, Sociability, and Rhythmicity, second-order factors extracted from the ATP data were labelled

Negative Emotionality, Self-Regulation, and Sociability. The Children's Behavior Questionnaire (CBQ) has also consistently produced three broad factors: 1) Surgency/Extraversion, is defined primarily by the scales of Approach, High Intensity Pleasure (sensation-seeking), Activity Level, and a negative contribution from Shyness. 2) Negative Affectivity, is defined by the scales of Discomfort, Fear, Anger/Frustration, Sadness, and loading negatively, Soot ability. 3) Effortful Control, is defined by the scales of Inhibitory Control, Attentional Focusing, Low Intensity Pleasure, and Perceptual Sensitivity. These three factors map well on the second-order factors identified by Sanson et al. (1994): Surgency/Extraversion on Sociability; Negative Affectivity on Negative Emotionality, and Effortful Control on Self-Regulation. From child to adult measures and simil
Dettagli
Publisher
A.A. 2020-2021
41 pagine
SSD Scienze storiche, filosofiche, pedagogiche e psicologiche M-PSI/01 Psicologia generale

I contenuti di questa pagina costituiscono rielaborazioni personali del Publisher sakuraxxx di informazioni apprese con la frequenza delle lezioni di Individual differences in temperament and personality e studio autonomo di eventuali libri di riferimento in preparazione dell'esame finale o della tesi. Non devono intendersi come materiale ufficiale dell'università Università degli Studi di Roma La Sapienza o del prof Di Giunta Laura.